Date: Thu, 16 Oct 1997 10:32:08 -0400
From: Gregory {Greg} Downing downingg[AT SYMBOL GOES HERE]IS2.NYU.EDU
Subject: Folk-Etymology (Was Re: Etymology of _Hoosier_)
At 09:22 AM 10/16/97 ("Dennis R. Preston" preston[AT SYMBOL GOES HERE]PILOT.MSU.EDU wrote:
What is it that makes what people believe not a fact?
It works for them, and/or it's the best they can figure out, or the best
they have heard. I'm not saying folk-etymology shouldn't exist. (I'm not
sitting at the edge of the sea telling the tide not to come in.) It will
always exist to some extent. What I am saying is it wouldn't hurt to
maintain a distinction between (1) the best available information on what
really happened in cultural and semantic history, and (2) fanciful but
culturally and personally resonant ideas about word-origins.
What makes what
people believe about language a set of facts not interesting to linguists?
They are, absolutely. In fact, not an inconsiderable component of what
exists in present language is, of course, the result of the effects of past
folk-etymolgies or word-confusions.
Finally, if you want to stamp some out
I was quoting Skeat, and arguing that if people want to think f-e's are
*really true*, they certainly have hold of (2) but are missing (1) -- which
is maybe fine in most cases, but probably not in the case of linguists or
lexicographers.
(and I agree that there are some
viscious folk beliefs about language, or, to be more precise, about
language users), wouldn't you want to know the details, sources, strength,
provenience, and so on of those beliefs before you went a-stompin'?
By all means. What's your theory as to why people want to think "hoosier"
comes from "who's here?"? Does it have something to do with popular ideas
about pioneer life (people calling out to each other in the woods etc.)?
The distinction between (1) and (2) above means you observe and analyze even
etymologies that are well-known to be inaccurate historically, but you don't
confuse (1) and (2).
There is also some caution to be taken in lessons learned from medicine and
other areas (which we all pray are dominated by hard science) where folk
facts have tuned out to be right on.
There are two things to be analyzed: what actually happened historically in
terms of phonetic and semantic evolution (which is sometimes clear and
sometimes not so clear), and what people think happened though it didn't
(I'm talking here only of cases where a particular etymology is
**demonstrably impossible** given the available historical record). When an
analyst is descriptivist about the *first* thing, s/he is trying to get as
close as possible to "wie es eigentlich gewesen," what really happened in
language-history. When an analyst is descriptivist about the *second* thing,
s/he is chronicling people's always imperfect imformation-levels, and the
effects that those information-levels have on the culture and the language
-- which is not the same as saying they are historically accurate, just that
in being believed by people they have effects that also have to be taken
account of in analyzing the actual history of language. Folk-etymologies are
linguistic facts to be studied *after* they start to become current, but not
facts at all if you're looking at the actual *earlier* development of the
word (and the latter is what folk-etymologies wrongly purport to explain).
When I was at the Univ. of Michigan (near your stomping ground) at the end
of the 1970s, everyone liked to say that "history" came from "his story,"
revealing the patriarchal background of the culture. Of course, that has
nothing to do with the word's Greek origin, so it will never be (to follow
your analogy above) a "folk remedy" that turns out in the end to be true
after all in explaining the word's origin. But the fact that people really
thought that does tell you what was on their minds and on the collective
culture's mind, i.e., the progress of modern feminism during the 1970s (when
I heard this etymology used a lot). And the fact that the etymology is
historically false does not falsify the insight that generated it: the
historical dominance of men in political and economic affairs, etc. The
accurate insight is what lured people into thinking the false etymology made
sense. But the fact that the insight was accurate doesn't make the etymology
accurate.
I work on Joyce's _Finnegans Wake_. Some of the puns are etymolgically
driven and thus reflect connections and patterns that are part of people's
long-term conceptual and cultural history as reflected in linguistic
history; they are "etymological" puns, so to speak. Other _FW_ paronomasia
comes from linkages of sound and sense that are not etymologically-based,
but are evocative of genuine patterns in reality or culture. So both
scientific etymology and other kinds of language-connections are useful. But
they are not the same thing. And for historical linguists and etymologists
the difference is their profession; a fair amount of what is posted on this
list is the attempt to determine actual origins or etymologies, not what
people might wrongly think the origins and etymologies are.
If folk-etymology is just as likely to be historically valid as more
"scientific" (i.e., empirically-based) etymology, Murray and Skeat and all
subsequent lexicographers could have saved themselves many decades of effort
by simply reprinting all the clever (the cleverer and more entertaining the
better!) but unhistorical folk-etymologies seen in Horne Tooke or in any
issue of Note & Queries in the mid 19th century, rather than looking into
how words actually originated and developed as best that can be determined
at any point in time.
Greg Downing/NYU, at greg.downing[AT SYMBOL GOES HERE]nyu.edu or downingg[AT SYMBOL GOES HERE]is2.nyu.edu